Even
before birth, humans demonstrate the ability to perceive sound and respond to
this stimulus by eliciting heart rate changes and movement (Abrams 1995). This
auditory perception may be useful in allowing newborn infants to recognize
their mother’s voice and quickly develop an intimate relationship (DeCasper and
Prescott 1984). With regards to music, mothers often communicate with infants
by exaggerating melodic contour, or the ups and downs of melody, to convey
emotional meaning even before the infant can understand their language (Fernald
et al. 1989). This communicative strategy simultaneously initiates the infant’s
musical development. Several perceptive mechanisms in music emerge from this
beginning: harmony, rhythm, pitch relations, scale structure, and
discrimination between consonance and dissonance (Winkler et al. 2009).
Some of
these mechanisms undergo unique development that mirror essential aspects of
human brain activity. For example, infants can actually outperform adults in
remembering artificial scales, due to adult enculturation of conventional
scales at the expense of unfamiliar ones (Trehub et al. 1999). Composers like
Debussy often employ similarly unfamiliar scales in their music, such as the
popular Claire de Lune.
Alternatively, another study comparing infants and adults discovered that both
groups prefer consonant intervals, suggesting that humans may possess a
congenital preference for consonance in music, irrespective of specific
development (Schellenberg and Trainor 1996). Investigations of brain activity
have even discovered specific brain regions that process consonance, regions also
involved in the emotional response (Blood et al. 1999). Perhaps this indicates
an innate, emotional attraction toward certain sounds in music. Other perceptive
mechanisms, such as pitch relations, have been manipulated to confuse
listener’s perception. One intriguing project, the Shepard scale, utilizes
ambiguous timbre and seemingly never-ending pitch height to arouse emotions
(Shepard 1964). This evoked emotional arousal occurs when expectancies of upcoming
pitches are not met (Huron 2006), and may further reflect the connection
between music and emotion seen in consonance.
Among
all the senses, sound has a unique power to arouse intense feelings. As
championed by philosopher John Dewey (1934), “sounds come from outside the
body, but sound itself is near, intimate; an excitation of the organism . . .
vision arouses emotion in the form of interest . . . it is sound that makes us
jump.” Music, or the organization of sound, is thus a robust way to convey
emotion and immerse oneself in feeling. Emotional contagion (the phenomenon that
perceiving an emotion induces the same emotion) occurs frequently in music to
dramatic effect: fast,
bright music with exaggerated rhythmic contrast may motivate the audience
to jovial action whereas a slow,
soft performance heavy in vibrato may generate sadness or longing (Thompson
2009). In addition to these psychophysical cues, music can trigger visual
images (e.g., a stormy night) that may include emotional connotations, or music
itself may remind someone of an emotionally significant memory (e.g., a
romantic evening) (Juslin and Sloboda 2013). These evoked emotions, whether
psychophysical, multisensory, or relational, sometimes differ between the
sexes.
Sex
differences in musical perception, especially related to emotion, are
intriguing despite a lack of consensus by scholars. For example, a study in
which children were subjected to harmonious and inharmonious chord progressions
yielded clear differences in electric brain potentials: boys showed lateralized
activation in the right brain hemisphere while girls demonstrated bilateral
activation (especially Figure 3C, Koelsch et al. 2003).
Alternatively, some researchers have uncovered opposite
mental recruitment in adult males (Koelsch et al. 2002) or actually increased
lateralization in women (Evers et al. 1999). These disparate results suggest
that during childhood development, neurological investment in musical
perception shifts – an indication of neuroplasticity. However, other studies
conclude that no sex difference in physiological responses (e.g., skin
conductance, finger temperature, heart rate, and facial expression) occurs during
exposure to emotionally powerful music (Lundqvist et al. 2009; Rickard 2004;
Robazza et al. 1994). Although this null result is compelling, their conclusion
seems questionable considering that they relied on physiological responses
instead of brain imaging. Thus, sex differences in the brain’s emotional
response may differ by sex even though the body’s responses to emotionally significant
music are similar in all people. Further research should explore a potential
connection between these lateralized activational studies and induced
physiological responses, so that a more complete portrayal of musical
perception and emotion can be understood. This future work may not only
identify sex differences in this pathway, but evaluate the influence of age,
cultural background, and musical training on experiencing the sound of music.
References
Abrams, R. 1995.
Some aspects of the fetal sound environment. In I. Deliege and J. Sloboda (eds.),
Perception and cognition of music (pp.
83-101). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
Blood, A., R.
Zatorre, P. Bermudez, and A. Evans. 1999. Emotional responses to pleasant and
unpleasant music correlate with activity in paralimbic brain regions. Nature Neuroscience 2: 382-387.
DeCasper, A. and
P. Prescott. 1984. Human newborns’ perception of male voices: preference,
discrimination, and reinforcing value. Developmental
Psychobiology 17: 481-491.
Dewey, J. 1934. Art as experience. New York: Minton,
Balch.
Evers, S., J.
Dannert, D. Rodding, G. Rotter, and E. Ringelst. 1999. The cerebral
haemodynamics of music perception. Brain 122:
75-85.
Fernald, A., et
al. 1989. A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers’ and
fathers’ speech to preverbal infants. Journal
of Child Language, 16: 477-501.
Huron, D. 2006. Sweet anticipation: music and the psychology
of expectation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Juslin, P., and
J. Sloboda. 2013. Music and Emotion. In Diana Deutsch (ed), The psychology of music (3rd
ed., pp. 583-645). London: Academic Press.
Koelsch, S., et
al. 2003. Children processing music: electric brain responses reveal musical
competence and gender differences. Journal
of Cognitive Neuroscience 15: 683-693.
Koelsch, S., B.
Maess, T. Grossmann, and A. Friederici. 2002. Sex difference in music-syntactic
processing. NeuroReport 14: 709-712.
Lundqvist, L., F.
Carlsson, P. Hilmersson, and P. Juslin. 2009. Emotional responses to music:
experience, expression, and physiology. Psychology
of Music 37: 61-90.
Rickard, N. 2004.
Intense emotional responses to music: a test of the physiological arousal
hypothesis. Psychology of Music 32:
371-388.
Robazza, C., C.
Macaluso, and V. D’Urso. 1994. Emotional reactions to music by gender, age, and
expertise. Perceptual and Motor Skills 79:
939-944.
Schellenberg, E.,
and L. Trainor. 1996. Sensory consonance and the perceptual similarity of
complex-tone harmonic intervals: tests of adult and infant listeners. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America
100: 3321-3328.
Shepard, R. 1964.
Circularity in judgments of relative pitch. Journal
of the Acoustical Society of America 36: 2345-2353.
Thompson, W.
2009. Music, thought, and feeling.
New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Trehub, S., E.
Schellenberg, and S. Kamenetsky. 1999. Infants’ and adults’ perception of scale
structure. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 25: 965-975.
Winkler, I., G.
Haden, O. Ladinig, I. Sziller, and H. Honing. 2009. Newborn infants detect the
beat in music. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences (USA) 106: 2468-2471.