The poem “Totally, Like, Whatever” by Taylor Mali made
the viral rounds several years ago and playfully criticizes the way younger
generations speak with one another. He mocks the conversion of declarative
sentences into questions, also known as “uptalk” (raised intonation at the end
of phrases), and the excessive use of “like” and “you know” that characterize
young people’s speech habits.
In the 1970’s, a new sociolect (a variety of a
language spoken by a certain social group) in Southern California called
“valleyspeak” or “valley girl” started making its way into popular culture. The
characteristics of this way of talking? Uptalk and excessive use of “like”.
Taylor Mali’s depiction of a generation’s language that is similar to a
sociolect of the 1970’s exhibits a phenomenon virtually unknown to the general
public: that women are the linguistic innovators that spark language evolution
for future generations of both males and females.
Linguists have documented this phenomenon, and
they are careful to state that such linguistic change is never indicative of a
language’s descent into a “lower quality” state. Dialects and languages are not
better structured than other variations; they are simply different and each has
its own complex set of rules. Documentation of uptalk between both genders and
descriptions of how both genders use it strategically help support the idea
that changes in language trends do not mean a lowering of a linguistic standard. So, Taylor Mali’s
criticism of the new way of speaking is linguistically flawed, and, radically
speaking, against women’s ways of talking in 1970’s southern California.
Setting sociolinguistics aside (whole books can
be written on this subject), it is trickier to explain why and how language
change occurs, and why women in particular are agents in this process. Studies
that investigate linguistic change over time are capable of describing what
happens and suggesting how women are able to instigate linguistic change and
innovation. An article in the New York Times summarizes some
theories; one, that women use language to assert their power in a culture that usually denies them agency, and two, that young women are socially permitted to experiment with language,
While linguistic and sociological studies can
describe what occurs on the surface, they cannot get into the deeper reasons
that may explain what determines this phenomenon. When I refer to “deeper
reasons”, I refer to the possibility that there could be structural features in
the female brain that may contribute to their roles as linguistic innovators in
society. A study conducted by Douglas D. Burman, Tali Bitan, and James R. Booth in 2008 attempts to explain
how men and women differ in language abilities. Using a technique called
functional magnetic resonance imaging, the researchers were able to identify
what parts of the brain are used when performing certain verbal tasks by
measuring the amount of oxygenated blood that flows to certain regions.
They found that girls use language-specific
structures in the brain and are more accurate when performing language tests,
whereas boys rely on other brain structures not related to language and perform
less accurately on the tests. The researchers conclude that girls “have
significantly greater activation in linguistic areas of the brain” than boys. This
study may explain why women are linguistic innovators: if women are more
efficient at learning and using language than men are, it is a logical
conclusion that women are more capable of introducing new linguistic ideas into
the population.
While this study presents sound methods and
reasonable analyses and conclusions on the data, it is always important to proceed
with caution when trying to attribute differences between the sexes and social
behavior to biological structures. Science has often been used to justify
existing gender biases and reinforce the idea that women are passive beings who
are subject to the changes placed upon them. However, with this study and the
idea that women have power when it comes to language change, it is possible to
rethink previous ideas about language and power, and expand our knowledge base
instead of reinforcing what is already believed. Embracing linguistic change as
an expansion, instead of rejecting it as a downgrade like Taylor Mali, supports
the idea that women can have power in society and help dictate the future of linguistic change.
Hi Athena!
ReplyDeleteGreat job on the article. I noticed that you reference structural changes in your post, when the paper you reference refers only to functional differences in brain activity. Which did you mean to emphasize? Overall, good post.
Caleb
Since structure and function are so closely intertwined, I meant to emphasize both. Boy's brains process language differently, so it suggests that there is a different neuronal makeup that sends messages to different areas of the brain. However, it is important to remember that "neural makeup" can get into some problematic phrasing that leads to people believing the brain is "hard-wired" for certain tasks, when in reality the brain is not static.
ReplyDeleteThanks for the comment and your feedback!